Proximity in agreement errors Barbara Hemforth and Lars Konieczny It is well known by now that occasionally subjects erroneously produce plural verbs following a plural modifier in constructions like (1; quoted form Bock & Miller, 1991; see Bock, 1995, for an overview). (1) The readiness of our conventional forces are at an all-time low. The mechanism underlying this error is attributed to the marked plural feature percolating up the tree too far (Vigliocco & Nicol, 1995). This account is substantiated by the fact that no comparable singular/plural mismatch effect for constructions with marked plural heads has been established so far. Very recently, Haskell and MacDonald (2002) proposed a principle of proximity as an alternative explanation. They showed that in disjunctions like (2), subjects have a strong preference to match the number marking on the verb with the more local noun. (2) a. The hat or the gloves is/are red. We ran a series of four written production experiments to test the proposed mechanisms in German. In all experiments subjects received a booklet with constructions missing an auxiliary which they had to fill in (see materials 1-16). In Experiment I, we established the usual asymmetry between singular and plural head nouns with a mismatch effect showing up for singular head nouns but not for plural head nouns. Similar to Branigan et al. (1995) and Hölscher & Hemforth (2000), more errors were found following plural head nouns but there was no effect of the number marking on the modifier. In Experiment II we could show that our subjects were highly sensitive to the number marking on the local noun in disjunctions, although there was a general tendency for plural verbs. The proximity principle assumed by Haskell and MacDonald clearly seems to be at work in disjunctions in German as well. In Experiments III and IV, we wanted to find out in how far the proximity
effect extends to different structures. In German sub-clauses the unmarked
ordering of constituents is subject < object < verb. We presented
our subjects with unambiguous orderings, disambiguated by case marking
in Experiment III and by plausibility in Experiment IV. If a close plural
marked noun has an effect on number marking on the verb, there should
be an increased number of errors in sentences like (10) or (14) where
a singular subject is followed by a plural object. However, no effect
of the number However, in contrast to Experiment I, we found a locality effect for sentences with plural subjects. There were more errors (incorrectly produced singular verbs) following a singular object noun phrase than after a plural object noun phrase. We will argue that this mismatch effect is due to the monitoring component, where the basic tendency to produce a singular verb even with plural subjects is inhibited by the plural marking on the object noun phrase. We are currently testing in how far these results extend to oral production. We are also running comprehension experiments (eyetracking), to see whether comprehension is affected by comparable principles. Examples: Experiment I Experiment II 5. Der Hut oder die Handschuhe _________ rot. 6.7 % singular The hat
or the gloves _________ red. Experiment III Experiment IV 13. Es ist unwahrscheinlich, dass die Krankenschwester die Spritze
vergessen _________. 4.4 % errors It is not probable that the nurse
the injection forgotten _________. |