Are
phonological phrases exploited on-line for the syntactic
analysis of spoken sentences?
Severine Millotte and Anne Christophe
Laboratoire de Sciences Cognitives et Psycholinguistique, CNRS-EHESS
Prosody has often been proposed as being useful for early syntactic
acquisition (e.g. Morgan, 1986). There is evidence that intonational
phrases constrain on-line syntactic analysis in adults (e.g. Warren
et al. 1995), and are perceived by infants. Here we focus on a smaller
unit, the phonological phrase. Recent evidence suggests that both adults
and
1-year-old infants interpret phonological phrase boundaries as word
boundaries on-line (Christophe et al. 2001). Since phonological phrases
are available early enough in processing to constrain lexical access,
they might also constrain early syntactic analysis. This is what we
tested here with adults.
We constructed pairs of sentences with a local syntactic ambiguity,
in which one word was either a verb or an adjective: "[Le petit
chien] [mord la laisse] [qui le retient]." "[Le petit chien
mort] [sera enterré demain].." ("[the little dog] [bites
the leash] [which ties him]" "[the little dead dog] [will
be buried tomorrow]"; square brackets mark phonological phrases).
Both sentences contain words which are pronounced identically until
the ambiguous word; however, they differ in syntactic structure, and
as a result in prosodic structure. The ambiguous word is preceded by
a phonological phrase boundary when it is a verb, but followed by one
when it is an adjective. In experiment 1, subjects completed
auditorily presented sentence fragments cut just after the ambiguous
word. They gave significantly more verb responses for verb sentences
than for adjective sentences (and vice-versa). This off-line experiment
tells us that phonological phrase boundaries are perceived and exploited
by adult listeners, but not when they are exploited: It could either
be during the
generation of syntactic parses or during selection (e.g. Boland et al.
2001). In experiment 2, subjects detected words defined with their syntactic
category (e.g. "mordre", "to bite" for the verb
target). They had to respond to the verb target in the verb sentence,
but refrain from responding in the adjective sentence. In addition,
non-ambiguous control
sentences contained the same target words. If prosodic boundary cues
were a perfect indicator of syntactic structure, we would observe no
effect of ambiguity whatsoever. We did observe slower responses for
ambiguous sentences. However, subjects false-alarmed more to adjective
sentences, where the prosodic boundary follows the target word, than
to verb
sentences, where the prosodic information comes earlier. This suggest
that prosody did play a role, even if it was not sufficient to entirely
disambiguate the sentences. A further experiment will use sentences
produced with a neutral prosody, in which prosodic disambiguating cues
are less reliable; We expect a greater effect of ambiguity in this condition.